Custodial Suicide
Suicide of an inmate/resident of a correctional or detention facility
Segen's Medical Dictionary.
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Custodial suicide: State to pay Rs 2 lakh to victim’s kin
Kanchan Chaudhari, Hindustan Times
Mumbai, August 06, 2011
Email to Author
First Published: 02:29 IST(6/8/2011)
Last Updated: 02:29 IST(6/8/2011)
Share more...
0 Comments
Email print
Ruling that there was a difference in a suicidal death in police custody and a suicide elsewhere, the Nagpur bench of the Bombay high court held that the former attracted tortuous liability to state. A division bench of justice AH Joshi and justice UV Bakre directed the state government
to pay Rs2 lakh to the family of Nitin Patil, 23, a resident of Kanhan village of Nagpur district, who allegedly committed suicide while in custody of the Ramtek police.
The compensation will carry interest at the rate of 6% per annum from July 23, 2003, the day Patil died.
The government had argued that suicidal death was too remote a cause for claiming compensation since it was not attributable to any wrong. The court, however, discarded the contention.
The bench also shifted the burden on the police to prove that the suicide was not due to torture or ill treatment meted out to the victim.
The police had labelled Patil a habitual offender after he was arrested in 2000 for a petty offence.
Patil shifted to Orissa and got married. However, when he visited his native place on July 11, 2003, the Kanhan police arrested him. Four different police stations registered six different offences against him.
Noting that four of the six cases were registered against unknown persons, the high court observed, “It [arrest] can be for motive, which smacks of being ulterior, at least it lacks due diligence, bonafides and basic respect to the rights of the citizens.”
Patil’s family members had sought Rs10 lakh compensation alleging the police brutality had driven him to commit suicide. The court, however, reduced it to Rs2 lakh.
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------
RETHINKING THE PROBLEM OF
CUSTODIAL SUICIDE
Daniel B. Kennedy, Ph.D.
University of Detroit Mercy, 4001 W. McNichols Rd., P O Box 19900
Detroit, MI 48219-3599, USA
Introduction
Several years ago, a 42 year old native Alaskan woman was arrested for public intoxication.
Because she was arrested in a small town, police were familiar with her recent history.
They knew that in the previous few months, one of her sons had been burned to death and
another had been stabbed to death. The woman's mother also had died and her husband had left
her. Because the woman had been screaming that someone should locate and check on her two
children, the door to the cell area was closed in order to muffle the noise. This facility had
experienced 15-20 suicide attempts during a calendar year, and CCTV placement was
inadequate. The woman committed suicide. The Supreme Court of Alaska ruled that a jury
could conclude that clinical judgment was not required for the police to have been on notice of
potential suicide (Kanayurak v. North Slope Borough, 677 P.2d 893, Alaska, 1984).
To date, no clinical test can reliably predict suicide without also identifying an
unacceptable number of false positives (inmates who are identified as suicidal but
who do not kill themselves). Some consultants have constructed lists of behavioral
characteristics which are associated with suicide ex post facto, but these
lists are not useful in a prospective sense.
In another case, a young man in North Dakota was involved in an auto accident and
arrested at the scene for driving while intoxicated. Because he "foul-mouthed" a Bismarck
police officer, he was placed in "the hole," a small cell which had four solid walls and contained
only a toilet bowl. The only lighting came from two small grill openings at the top and bottom
of the door, one of which could be covered by a flap. The cell was located in a noisy basement,
which also contained heating and other equipment. The use of "the hole" was generally limited
to unruly prisoners who were moved into regular cells once they had settled down. Young
Falkenstein was left in the cell all night and was found hanging from the cell door bars with his
AMERICAN JAILS January/February 1994
T-shirt knotted around his neck. The Supreme Court of North Dakota upheld a lower court
judgment stating that there was "substantial evidence to support a conclusion that (the) suicide
was the result of a morbid state of mind proximately caused by incarceration in `the hole' for an
extended period of time" (Falkenstein v. City of Bismarck, 268 N.W. 2d 787, N.D., 1978).
In both of these cases, reasonable persons could conclude that the suicides were foreseeable
and, therefore, that precautions to prevent the suicides should have been taken. It is my
position, however, that many, if not most, custodial suicides do not involve such "obvious"
circumstances and, in fact, are not reasonably foreseeable. For example, a young man who has
had a minor scrape with the law while drinking might find himself incarcerated. Within hours
and without any warning, he may kill himself for reasons, which can never be known. Such
custodial suicides are spontaneous in nature and do not display characteristics which clearly
serve as signals to corrections personnel. Nevertheless, some juries and even corrections officers
themselves may tend to believe the state, county, or municipality was negligent in "allowing"
such suicides to occur. As will be shown, this tendency may be more the result of errors in
reasoning than illustrative of negligence. More specifically, corrections officers must understand
the unpredict able nature of many suicides, and they must come to grips with the "evil-causesevil
fallacy."
The Unpredictable Nature of Custodial Suicides
Statistically speaking, suicide in custody is a rare phenomenon, and rare phenomena are
notoriously difficult to forecast due to their low base rate. Note also that the term "forecast" is
preferred rather than the term "predict." We cannot predict suicide because social scientists are
not fully aware of the causal variables involving suicide. Without sufficient knowledge of causeeffect
relationships among variables, prediction is not possible (Nettler, 1978).
To illustrate the statistically rare nature of custodial suicide, certain assumptions must be
made. Although far fewer suicides per year have been documented, perhaps 450 or so (Hayes,
1989), I shall assume as have some authors that the real figure is closer to 1,000 (Charlé, 1981).
If we assume that only 10,000,000 of the approximately 14,200,000 persons arrested each year
spend any time behind bars, then only one out of 10,000 persons at risk of custodial suicide
actually commits suicide. Put another way, if the average daily jail population is 408,000, and
there are three suicides in custody each day (again, a most liberal estimate), then the chances of a
prisoner killing himself on any given day are one out of 136,000. If we add state and federal
prisoners to the picture, there are 1.2 million people behind bars at any given time. The chances
of a prisoner committing suicide now become one out of 400,000! Even in a psychiatric setting
involving patients who are clinically depressed and diagnosed as suicide risks, relatively few
actually kill themselves. The actual suicide rates for such desperate populations range from a
low of one percent to a high of only seven percent over a period of up to ten years (Motto, 1980;
Motto, Heilbron and Juster, 1985). Unfortunately, neither corrections officers nor even psychiatrists
are possessed of such precise diagnostic skills as to allow them to select the one individual
who will kill himself out of so many who might.
It is well established that psychologists, psychiatrists, and sociologists are unable to
forecast suicides in any practical sense (Murphy, 1983; Pokorny, 1983). To date, no clinical test
AMERICAN JAILS January/February 1994
can reliably predict suicide without also identifying an unacceptable number of false positives
(inmates who are identified as suicidal but who do not kill themselves). Some consultants have
constructed lists of behavioral characteristics, which are associated with suicide ex post facto,
but these lists are not useful in a prospective sense. In other words, knowing that an inmate is
depressed does not help to determine if he will kill himself since most inmates are somewhat
depressed by their situation, at least at first, and do not kill themselves. Being agitated and/or
intoxicated are not predictive of suicide, although they may be postdictive. These lists of
"warning signs" are more useful in telling us what did happen, not what will happen. Demographic
profiles of custodial suicide victims are also of little predictive validity (Kennedy and
Homant, 1988).
Recent surveys of practicing psychiatrists indicate that perhaps 51 percent have experienced
the suicide of a patient under their care (Chemtob et al., 1988). This sad fact suggests that
even those skilled mental health professionals, who have the time for extensive personal interaction
with troubled individuals either cannot forecast suicide or are unable to prevent patient
suicide even if it had been somewhat anticipated. Indeed, as more and more studies show that
mental health professionals often fail to reach reliable or valid conclusions (Faust and Ziskin,
1988), some plaintiff's experts are telling jurors that almost all custodial suicides can be
prevented. The fact is, of course, that many suicides can be prevented if corrections officers
have reason to forecast that a given individual is likely to kill himself. But even if the suicide
attempt of an individual is forecast, the only certain method of preventing him from committing
suicide is to station an officer within arm's reach at all times (Atlas, 1989). Given the strong
probability that even this inmate will not kill himself, the manpower dilemma in which many
small departments find themselves becomes obvious.
Contrary to the questionable notion that proper interviewing will generally elicit truthful
answers from individuals in the booking process, many individuals who kill themselves probably
do not know they intend to do so when asked their intentions. Furthermore, if they truly did wish
to die, why would they tell this to the booking officer since to do so would make it more difficult
for them to harm themselves? Finally, how truthful are inmates anyway? In one study wherein
detainees were screened twice within 72 hours, only 48 percent made totally consistent responses
to the same question and 33 percent made one inconsistent response (Sherman and Morschauser,
1989).
Custodial suicides are extremely difficult to forecast due to their spontaneous nature,
particularly where younger inmates are concerned. For example, family physicians who have
recently been in contact with suicided adolescents rarely detect sufficient signals to anticipate
self-destruction (Teicher and Jacobs, 1966). Studies in English prisons suggest that in at least 50
percent of the cases, suicide was performed on a sudden impulse (Topp, 1979). It is because
suicide is so impulsive (Murphy, 1983; Salive, Smith and Brewer, 1989; Williams, Davidson and
Montgomery, 1980) that it is so hard to forecast. Corrections workers cannot be expected to
know of an individual's self-destructive intentions if the inmate himself is not yet aware of them.
The "Evil Causes Evil Fallacy"
Several decades ago, noted criminologist Albert Cohen described what he called the
"evil-causes-evil fallacy." In essence, this is the belief that bad consequences must have had bad
AMERICAN JAILS January/February 1994
causes. Thus, if something bad such as a suicide occurs in a jail, then somebody must have been
negligent. If a young man turns out to be antisocial, then his parents must have reared him
improperly. If a given football team does poorly in competition, then it must have had poor
coaching, and so forth. The fact of the matter is, of course, that many things happen for reasons
beyond human control. Sociopathic behavior can occur as a result of delayed development of the
Reticular Activating System, a biological problem (Bartol, 1991). Football teams may fail
because of pure skill limitations, independent of coaching.
The need that humans have to attribute causation to phenomena may be explained by
attribution theory (Goldstein, 1980). Essentially, people have a need to explain things, even if
their explanations are wrong. Attribution does serve a number of purposes. For example, by
identifying the "causes" of a tragedy such as a custodial suicide, we tell ourselves that we are
now in control of our environment, and therefore our future. Our world becomes a safer place to
be, even at the price of blaming ourselves for something which was not our fault. Even
traumatized children have created false explanations for their tragedies as a mechanism by
which the "breached ego" attempts retrospectively to regain control and mastery (Terr, 1981).
Unfortunately, misplaced attribution by corrections workers themselves can lead to
inappropriate guilt and even liability as a consequence of custodial suicide. Not long ago, I
examined a jail suicide on behalf of a county sheriff being sued vicariously for the death of an
inmate. The inmate had a history of alcoholism and suicidal ideation. He asked for counseling
assistance. A graduate MSW with several years experience was assigned the case and immediately
placed the inmate on a suicide watch in the medical cellblock. The social worker helped
the inmate resolve his immediate anxiety. The inmate started a diary, was reenrolled in the jail
AA program and expressed satisfaction with his own progress. The inmate asked to be
transferred to a newer part of the jail providing podular, direct supervision.
After documenting the inmate's progress thoroughly, the social worker made a conscious
decision to place the inmate back in the general population. Consistent with current
psychotherapeutic theory concerning the treatment of suicide, she believed the salubrious
benefits of the less restrictive setting would outweigh the possibility of acute relapse (Bongar,
1991). The inmate hanged himself soon after being taken off suicide watch, and the family sued.
The social worker was plagued with guilt feelings. Unconsciously, she assumed she
exercised poor professional judgment leading to a man's death. "After all," her unconscious
mind told her, "you must have made a negligent decision because he did kill himself." The
proper test of professional competency is whether she made a reasonable decision given all the
facts she knew at the time the decision was made. The ultimate outcome is not the sole measure
of the quality of the decision. In this case, the social worker's decision at the time was perfectly
appropriate, and a jury agreed. Nevertheless, this perfectly competent and dedicated mental
health provider suffered unjust feelings of guilt for a long time after this tragedy.
It may be, of course, that attribution theory only explains a part of the "evil-causes-evil
fallacy." Years ago, Elizabeth Kubler-Ross described five stages through which people were
believed to pass as they were dying: denial, anger, bargaining, depression, and acceptance
(Kubler-Ross, 1969). Subsequent scholars have posited the existence of a similar pattern, which
AMERICAN JAILS January/February 1994
describes the reactions of survivors to the death of persons with whom they have been affectively
involved in some way. According to Kavanaugh (1974), Rando (1984) and Stone (1972), guilt
feelings are a natural part of the bereavement process. When corrections officers display a sense
of guilt over the death of an inmate, jurors in civil trials may tend to take such guilt as evidence
of professional negligence in the care of the deceased prisoner. Corrections officers and the
attorneys who defend them must take precautions to distinguish between guilt as a natural
element of the grieving process and feelings of guilt generated by actual professional
malpractice.
Conclusions
As we have seen, suicide by an inmate is not a matter, which can easily be forecast. In
hindsight, some deaths could have been foreseen due to the prisoners' obvious despondency,
prior history, and current self-destructive ideation. It is unlikely, however, that most suicidal
inmates broadcast such strong signals. As an impulsive act, suicide is an inherently difficult act
to forecast or predict. The statistical rarity of suicide in the prisoner population complicates the
problem of anticipating suicides even further.
The "evil-causes-evil fallacy" suggests that bad outcomes must have had bad antecedents.
Even corrections officers with near perfect service records may tend to look at themselves as
contributing to the death of an inmate. Such guilt is often the result of the need that humans have
to attribute suicide causation to something concrete, which they can understand. Guilt is also a
natural companion of grief and must not be taken automatically as a sign that, indeed, one was
professionally negligent. These concepts in no way absolve the corrections officer from the duty
to watch closely for the signs of suicide. If a suicide can be foreseen, it can often be prevented
through appropriate measures. Still, corrections officers are not mind readers, and they must not
be told after the fact that they should have been.
AMERICAN JAILS January/February 1994
References
Atlas, R. (1989). "Reducing the Opportunity for Inmate Suicide: A Design Guide." Psychiatric
Quarterly 60: 161-171.
Bartol, C. (1991). Criminal Behavior: A Psychosocial Approach 3rd ed. Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice-Hall.
Bongar, B. (1991). The Suicidal Patient: Clinical and Legal Standards of Care. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Charlé, S. (1981). "How to Stop Jail Suicides." Police Magazine 4 (June): 49-55.
Chemtob, C., R. Hamada, G. Baver, B. Kinney, and R. Torigoe. (1988). "Patients' Suicides:
Frequency and Impact on Psychiatrists." American Journal of Psychiatry 145: 224-228.
Faust, D., and J. Ziskin. (1988). "The Expert Witness in Psychology and Psychiatry." Science
241: 31-35.
Goldstein, J. (1980). Social Psychology. New York: Academic Press.
Hayes, L. (1989). "National Study of Jail Suicides: Seven Years Later." Psychiatric Quarterly
60: 7-29.
Kavanaugh, R. (1974). Facing Death. Baltimore: Penguin.
Kennedy, D., and R. Homant. (1988). "Predicting Custodial Suicides: Problems With the Use
of Profiles." Justice Quarterly 5: 441-456.
Kubler-Ross, E. (1969). On Death and Dying. New York: Macmillan.
Motto, J. (1980). Social Psychology. New York: Academic Press.
Motto, J., D. Heilbron, and R. Juster. (1985). "Development of a Clinical Instrument to
Estimate Suicide Risk." American Journal of Psychiatry 142: 680-686.
Murphy, F. (1983). "On Suicide Prediction and Prevention." Archives of General Psychiatry
40: 343-344.
Nettler, G. (1978). Explaining Crime. 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Pokorny, A. (1983). "Prediction of Suicide in Psychiatric Patients." Archives of General
Psychiatry 40: 249-257.
Rando, T. (1984). Grief, Dying and Death: Clinical Interventions for Caregivers. Champaign,
IL: Research Press.
Salive, M., G. Smith, and T. Brewer. (1989). "Suicide Mortality in the Maryland State Prison
System, 1979 Through 1987." Journal of the American Medical Association 262: 365-
569.
Sherman, L., and P. Morschauser. (1989). "Screening for Suicide Risk in Inmates." Psychiatric
Quarterly 60: 119-138.
Stone, H. (1972). Suicide and Grief. Philadelphia: Fortress Press.
Teicher, J., and J. Jacobs. (1966). "The Physician and the Adolescent Suicide Attempter."
Journal of School Health 36: 406-415.
Terr, L. (1981). "Psychic Trauma in Children: Observations Following the Chowchilla School-
Bus Kidnapping." American Journal of Psychiatry 138: 14-19.
Topp, D. (1979). "Suicide in Prison." British Journal of Psychiatry 134: 24-27.
Williams, C., J. Davidson, and I. Montgomery. (1980). "Impulsive Suicidal Behavior."
Journal of Clinical Psychology 36: 90-94.
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------
Custodial suicide in Australia: a comparative study of different populations.
Morrison S.
Source
Office of Strategic Crime Assessments, Commonwealth Law Enforcement Board, Parkes, Australia.
Abstract
Research on custodial suicide has run into immense difficulties in its attempt to identify common characteristics of the suicide-prone inmate. In 1993, Haycock recognized these difficulties and called for comparative research to delineate the different risks posed by different sorts of confined populations. This paper contrasts the characteristics of different categories of confined inmates based on data collected by the Australian Institute of Criminology on deaths in custody within Australia between the years 1980 and 1993, inclusive. This form of research will lead to a greater understanding of the phenomenon of custodial suicide and the establishment of better preventive strategies tailored to the needs of specific groups and organizations.
PMID:
8999280
[PubMed - indexed for MEDLINE]
No comments:
Post a Comment